Visual
Displays
When
a bird moves or holds itself in a way that signals
information to another bird of the same or different
species, it is said to be performing a "display." Thus a
pigeon leaping into flight at the sight of a hawk, only
coincidentally alerting other members of its flock in the
process, is moving but not displaying. In contrast, a male
grouse strutting on a lek (a traditional courting ground) is
displaying -- passing information about its desirability as
a mate to females of its own species. Displays may include
vocalizations, as they do in the case of the grouse -- and
in the broad sense, vocalizations alone are displays. But
because ornithologists often consider vocalizations
separately, for our discussion we will define as displays
only ritualized movements or postures.
Displays are usually
classified according to their apparent function: courtship,
aggression, begging, greeting, and so on. The strutting male
grouse is performing one of the myriad kinds of courtship
display seen in birds. Often courtship displays accent a
striking feature of the bird's plumage. The conspicuous,
labored flight displays of the male Red-winged Blackbird
exaggerate its red shoulder patches. The display flight of
the male Yellow-headed Blackbird is performed with the body
cocked upward so that its prominent yellow head is held
high.
On the other hand, some male
birds do not advertise with physical attributes; they
demonstrate skills. Male terns court females by displaying a
fresh-caught fish. Courting male European Gray Herons
perform ritualized hunting movements, erecting head
feathers, pointing their bills downward and clashing their
mandibles together. Many male passerines, when courting,
also lower their bills as if pecking at something below
them. Perhaps, next to singing, the most common component of
courtship displays in male songbirds is vibration of the
wings; other components include fluffing of the body
feathers, bill raising, thrusting the head forward, and
running using short steps.
Birds also use a great
diversity of agonistic displays (those used in threats and
actual combat). Male Mourning Doves may bow repeatedly and
then lift their heads and coo when defending their
territories. Canada Geese often pump their heads up and down
just before attacking. Male Gray Catbirds, disputing the
boundary between their territories, will fluff their
feathers, spread and often lower their tails, and as a last
resort, raise their wings. In the same situation Eastern
Kingbirds spread their tails to display their white terminal
band. Similarly, Tufted Titmice assume a horizontal posture,
may open their bills while slightly spreading their wings,
and lunge at the intruder. Aggressive geese may rear up and
spread their wings when on land; aggressive loons rear up in
the water. In established dominance hierarchies, dominant
birds often use threat displays against subordinates.
Subordinates signal their submission with other displays --
in passerines often by crouching with feathers fluffed and
head withdrawn.
Quite different from these
sexual and agonistic displays are begging displays, which
are employed both by chicks to solicit parental feeding and
by some females to solicit courtship feeding. Greeting
displays are used when one parent relieves the other at the
nest, and may serve to prevent aggressive interactions.
Pairs of adult Adelie Penguins do bowing displays and
exchange vocal greetings at "changings of the guard," thus
making large colonies extremely noisy. And, finally, there
are social displays that apparently help to keep flocks
unified. Displays are, in fact, a major part of the "glue"
that binds avian societies together.
Named elements
of the courtship display of the Western Grebe.
Clockwise from upper left:rushing, weed-dancing,
dip-shaking and bob-preening.
|
How this glue evolved
remains a matter of conjecture and dispute. It was the
courtship display of the Great Crested Grebe (which is much
like that of the Western Grebe illustrated above) that led
the pioneering British behaviorist and evolutionist Julian
Huxley to develop the concept of "ritualization" -- the
gradual evolutionary transformation of an everyday movement
into an increasingly effective signal. For instance,
preflight movements -- crouching, slight spreading of the
wings, and raising of the tail -- have been modified in many
birds into signals. When wooing a female, the male Great
Cormorant begins an exaggerated takeoff leap, but does not
leave the ground.
One of the most interesting
ideas on the origins of display behavior was developed in
the middle of this century by the celebrated ethologist
(student of behavior in natural environments) Niko Tinbergen
and his colleagues. Tinbergen suggested that internal
conflicts between different behavioral systems or tendencies
-- such as the desire to threaten and the urge to court --
are responsible for the generation of displays.
For instance, after
vigorously defending its territory, a male bird may approach
a female with ambivalence over whether to attack or woo her.
As a result, he may do neither, but instead channel his
energies into behavior that is irrelevant to either
aggression or mating -- say pulling at a tuft of grass or
preening. Such "displacement activities" can then become
incorporated into courtship displays, and "emancipated" from
their previous functions. Thus when displacement preening
becomes part of courtship, it also becomes more conspicuous
and stereotyped than normal preening, and becomes useful as
a signal, not as an aid in maintaining the bird's feathers.
Similarly, males of many species have incorporated
components of courtship fighting (battles between rivals
during the courtship season) into their courtship displays.
For example, among passerines, bill raising is used widely,
in some species in fighting, in others in courtship, and in
a few species in both. In many finches the preliminary male
courtship display is a modified head-forward threat
posture.
Although some behaviorists
think that virtually all displays arise from conflicts
between internal tendencies, experiments have so far failed
to confirm this. As a result, scientists are now turning
away from Tinbergen's approach, and looking at the possible
evolution of displays without reference to conflicting
"underlying tendencies" in the nervous system. Evolution has
modified an enormous variety of activities into displays,
from food exchange, as in courtship feeding (originating as
food exchange between parent and chick), to stylized fishing
movements used in heron courtship. We expect the
perspectives of those researchers investigating the overt
behavior of the animal and those studying the functioning of
its nervous system to gradually converge. That convergence,
we hope, will supply a deeper understanding of displays and
other behavioral phenomena.
SEE: Territoriality;
Distraction
Displays;
Dominance
Hierarchies;
Shorebird
Communication;
Duck
Displays;
American
Coots;
Redwing
Coverable Badges.
Copyright
® 1988 by Paul R. Ehrlich, David S. Dobkin, and Darryl
Wheye.
|